FrLOOGerls
Volume 1, Number 5 December/January 1972/1973 Price .35
a
Published by the Friends of the National Zoo
Published by
Friends of the National Zoo
National Zoological Park
Washington, D.C. 20009
Phones:
Executive Director and membership:
232-4500
Educational and Editorial offices:
232-5500
Guided tours: 332-9322
Window Shop: 232-4555
FONZ Board of Directors 1972-73
Peter C. Andrews — President
Arthur W. Arundel — Vice President
Emanuel Boasberg — Treasurer
Susan K. Semple — Secretary
Theodore Babbitt
Montgomery S. Bradley
John S. Brown
Mary H. L. Clark
Timothy V.A. Dillon
Donna K. Grosvenor
Stephen Hosmer
Joan L. Jewett
Robert Mason
Isabel J. McDonnell
Shirley ]. McNair
Lavelle Merritt
Ruth N. Nelson
John B. Oliver
Mary Poole
Nancy Porter
Gerald G. Wagner
Rosa M. Walker
Richardson White, Jr.
Executive Director
Warren J. Iliff
Editor: Austin Hughes
Design and Production by Tom Jones
Photographs on pages 7, 14, and 15 and on
back cover by Kathy Rasenberger. Cover
photograph and all other photographs by
Tom Jones.
THE ZOOGOER is published bi-monthly
and copyrighted © by Friends of the National
Zoo, c/o National Zoological Park, Washington,
D.C. 20009. second-class mailing permit
approved at Washington, D.C. Rate
in the United States $3 a year. Vol. 1, No. 4.
Hand Raising a Baby Gorilla
Zoo News — Mammals
Zoo Map
Zoo News — Birds
Zoo News — Reptiles and Amphibians
The Hoopoe
ine
10 © COnr
THE ZOOGOER is a new bi-monthly publication
of the Friends of the National Zoo and replaces
the quarterly publication, Spots and Stripes. With
THE ZOOGOER, the Friends hopes to provide
for its membership an up-to-date report on new
animals and exhibits at the National Zoological
Park and in-depth articles on the natural history
and behavior of animals at the Zoo. All of the text
and photographs will be keyed to the centerfold
map in hopes of making a trip to the Zoo a more
interesting, educational, and enjoyable adventure.
THE ZOOGOER will also be on sale at the Friends’
Window Shop and kiosk as a current guide to
points of interest at the Zoo.
Hand Raising
Bab
When Femelle, the National Zoo’s female
gorilla, gave birth to her first offspring
last May 29th, she appeared confused and
unable to care for the infant; so, rather
than risk its health or safety, Zoo officials
decided to remove the young gorilla for
hand raising. Chosen for the task were
Headkeeper of Small Mammals and Prima-
tes Bernie Gallagher and his wife Louise,
who had previously raised a total of
eight great ape young in their home. The
infant, a male, was given the name “Mgeni-
Mopaya’”’ by officials of the Embassy of
Zayre, but his caretakers usually refer to
him by the nickname “Mopie.’’ Mopie
will be seven months old in January; and
he is expected to return to his birthplace,
the Zoo’s Small Mammal Building
(number 15 on map), soon after the first
of the year.
When Mopie first arrived at the Gallagher’s
house, he slept most of the time. He was
fed a cow’s milk formula from a bottle
every two hours around the clock. His
eyes apparently could not focus for the
first few days, but after the third day
they began to be able to pick out and
follow moving objects in their surroundings.
Like other young gorillas, Mopie often
followed a moving object with his eyes
only but kept his head still. At rest his
eyes appeared entirely brown; but when
he moved them along with an object he
was watching, large amounts of white
appeared, giving the eye an appealingly
human appearance.
As do all newborn non-human primates,
gorilla infants have a strong clinging
reflex from birth. Although a newborn
gorilla’s grip is relatively weak compared
to that of a new born chimpanzee of
orangutan, from the first day Mopie could
support his own weight for a few seconds
clinging to one of his human caretakers’
fingers by only one hand. He tended to
cling to his foster mother’s left shoulder
while supported by her left forearm. This
preferred position is reminiscent of the
position in which a wild gorilla infant
clings to its mother when she is sitting
upright.
When the mother gorilla is sitting, the
newborn infant usually clings to her chest
where the nipples are located and is
supported by one of her forearms. As
it does so it may be partially shielded by
one or both of her arms and almost
entirely hidden by the long hair that
covers them. On the other hand, when the
mother is walking in the gorilla’s normal
quadrupedal fashion, for the first month
of life the infant usually clings across her
chest with its hands grasping the hair at
one side of her body and its feet grasp-
ing the hair at the opposite side. At this
age it is evidently unable to support its
own weight long in this position, and the
mother supports it almost constantly with
One arm as she uses the other one in
walking. When it is about a month old,
it often clings with its legs wrapped
around her waist and its feet grasping the
hair at the sides of her abdomen; in this
position, too, it generally requires support
from the mother.
At the age of about six or seven weeks
the wild gorilla infant is able to climb up
onto its mother’s back and ride there
lying prone while she is moving. It often
raises its head to look around and evident-
ly takes some interest in its surroundings.
Soon afterwards it is able to cling for
sometime to her underside without
support, but riding on the mother’s back
continues to be its preferred means of
transportation. By the time it is ten weeks
old it is leaving its mother for short
periods of time and can crawl slowly
behind her as she walks. In this sort of
crawling, the back legs are not straightened
out as they would be in the full quad-
rupedal gait of the adult. It is at the age
of about four months that gorilla infants
in the wild are reported to take their first
steps in the typical gorilla quadrupedal
fashion with both front and back limbs
straight and the hands held in such a way
that the weight rests on the knuckles.
Mopie has shown a comparable gradual |
development of his locomotory abilities.
In fact he was usually somewhat ahead of
the schedule reported for gorillas in the
wild. At the age of about one month, he
was able to move in a prone position by
a kind of swimming motion from one end
of the Gallaghers’ sofa to the other and
to pull himself up over a pillow or other
obstacle in this path. This kind of locomo-
tion probably showed the development
of the same motor abilities that enable
a wild gorilla infant to climb up onto
its mother’s back. By the age of six weeks,
Mopie was able to crawl. He had mastered
the quadrupedal gait by the surprisingly
early age of three months. By contrast the
three other gorillas the Gallaghers have
raised — born to a previous Zoo female —
were unable to walk in this manner until
the age of five months.
One of Mopie’s means of locomotion does
not seem to have been reported before.
In this form of progression, his back legs
are straight as they would be if he was
walking on all fours, but he leans forward
on his elbows. He pushes himself forward
by means of his back legs, but his arms
are held rigid. He moves this way when
he is playing, and he may be holding a
ball or other play object in his hands as
he does so.
By the age of five months, Mopie was
intensely active in play, climbing on furni-
ture, doing somersaults, and biting objects.
When exceptionally happy and excited he
would run on all fours. It was in similar
states of pleased excitement that Mopie
first showed one well-known gorilla behav-
ior pattern. Moving quadrupedally, he
stopped, raised one hand, and beat his
chest vigorously.
Mopie’s first milk tooth appeared at the
age of six weeks. In the case of Tomoka,
the first National-Zoo-born gorilla and the
first gorilla the Gallaghers raised, the first
milk tooth appeared even earlier, at the
age of 20 days. In human infants, by
contrast, the first milk tooth appears
between the third and sixteenth months,
with the average appearance between the
sixth and eighth months. At the age of
five months, Mopie already had ten teeth, —
six upper and four lower, and two more
lower molars were expected to appear
soon. Like a human baby, the young
gorilla would sometimes put his finger into
5
his mouth to rub his gum while teething.
Mopie was using his ten teeth to eat a
considerable amount of solid food. He was
often given apples and grapes, and un-
cooked stringbeans were a special favorite.
In addition, since he had been one month
old, he had been eating semi-solid baby
foods, including applesauce, strained vege-
tables, custard, and cereal. He was still
being bottle-fed formula, but the Galla-
ghers were gradually trying to increase the
number of hours he could go without
a bottle at night. At the age of five
months, he was able to go about seven
hours without crying for his bottle; but
when he returns to the Small Mammal
House, a bottle left with him at 4:30 p.m.
when the keepers leave will last
him until they return at 8:00 the next
morning.
It has been said that, except for the much
slower rate of brain growth, a gorilla infant
develops at approximately twice the rate
at which a human infant develops; and
the records kept of Mopie and other
National-Zoo-born gorillas have indicated
that this generalization holds for a variety
of aspects of the young gorilla’s develop-
ment, both physical and behavioral. Mopie,
who weighed five pounds ten ounces at
birth is the largest and fastest-growing
gorilla the Gallaghers have raised. At five
months he weighed fifteen pounds, five
ounces—almost three times his birth weight.
Human babies, which of course usually
weigh several pounds more at birth than
Mopie did, triple their birth weight only
by the age of one year. Interestingly,
at the age of five months even Mopie’s
sleep pattern closely resembled that of a
year-old human infant; at that time he
slept about eight hours at night and took
one-hour nap both in the morning and
in the afternoon.
In addition, gorillas reach sexual maturity
approximately twice as early as human
beings do, at the age of six or seven, and
full maturity at the age of about twelve
years. However, in many respects — includ-
ing the eruption of the milk teeth and |
the development of physical coordination
and motor abilities — the gorilla’s growth
is even more than twice as fast as a
human’s. And the seven-month-old Mgeni-
Mopaya that visitors will see at the Small
Mammal House in January will be
considerably more active, independent,
and self-assured than a human infant of
twice that age.
6
Greater Kudu Calves
All three of the Zoo’s female greater kudus
gave birth within one week of each other
at the beginning of October. One of the
calves is a male, and two are females; all
of them were sired by the Zoo’s one
magnificent spiral-horned adult male. Soon
afterwards the male was moved to a new
enclosure (number 9f on map) where he
has now been joined by the rest of the
greater kudu herd. These births bring to
13 the number of calves of this species
(Tragelaphus strepsicerus) born at the
National Zoo since 1968.
In the wild, a mother of this African
species keeps her newborn offspring hidden
night and day in a safe place for the first
several weeks of its life. The mother
approaches the hiding place only to nurse
her offspring, and at least for the first
week or two of its life the calf never
strays from where he has been left by the
mother. Similar behavior occurs in a great
many other species of antelope and in
many other hoofed mammals. Doubtless
it serves not only to safeguard the
helpless young from predators but also to
ensure the mother’s safety, since she is
One of the Zoo’s three new greater kudu calves
(number 9f on map).
freed from the necessity of staying in one
place with her young at all times, which
might endanger her as well. In keeping
with these habits, each of the Zoo’s greater
kudu mothers chose a hiding place for
her calf. One was placed in the moat at
the lower end of the kudus’ former enclo-
sure, while the other two were hidden
behind the large logs that lie at the upper
end of the enclosure.
For the first week of life, if a keeper app-
roached one of these hiding places, the
calf did not budge. After that, the young
would get up and run away if its hiding
place was approached. For this reason, the
adult male was kept in a holding pen away
from the females and their offspring
during the first week of each calf’s life;
for if he happened to stumble on one of
the calves’ hiding places before the calf
was able to protect itself by running away
he might injure one of his offspring. After
the first week, the calves still remained in
their hiding places much of the time, but
they also frequently left to frisk around and
explore the enclosure.
Since only one of the recent calves is a
male, he was the only one born with the
7
“buttons” that will later grow into horns.
At birth these ‘‘buttons” are bones that
have developed below the skin of the
forehead that are not connected with the
rest of the skeleton. Soon after birth
these bones fuse with the frontal — or
‘forehead’? — bones of the skull. Then,
very gradually, they begin to grow; and
the skin above them begins to produce
a horny outer sheath to cover them as
they grow. The growth of the male
greater kudu’s horns is very slow compared
to horn-growth in some other antelope
such as the scimitar-horned oryx (Oryx tao).
There are no noticeable horns in the male
greater kudu until he is eight or nine
months old; and when he is sexually
mature at the age of a year and a half,
his horns are only six or eight inches long.
They continue to grow slowly throughout
the animal’s life, reaching a length of
about 40 inches in a straight line or 52
inches measured along the curves. The
horns of one greater kudu measured
72 inches in a straight line.
Like the horns of other members of the
family Bovidae, which includes not only
antelope but also sheep, goats, oxen, and
bison, the greater kudu’s horns find their
primary function in combats between
males of the same species over herds of
females at the onset of mating season.
But in this species, in which the horns
have achieved perhaps their greatest
elaboration in any of the Bovidae, they
also play an important role as social
signals, particularly in courtship. A male
greater kudu initiates his courtship of the
female by stepping in her path and tilting
his head back and away from her, in effect
displaying his horns to her. Since the
horns grow gradually throughout life, it is
possible that a male who is fully mature
and therefore a better breeder than a
younger male can indicate this fact to the
female by displaying his horns to her and
thus enhance the effect his courtship has
on her.
Oryx Pair Sent to Haibar, Israel
The National Zoological Park has had consi-
derable success in recent years in breeding
the scimitar-horned oryx (Oryx tao), a
rare and endangered species of African
antelope. Nine offspring have been born
here since 1967, when an adult male and
two adult females were acquired for the
oepning of the new Delicate Hoofed Stock
Building (number 8c on map). The most
The rare and endangered scimitar-horned oryx
(number 8c on map).
recent was a male born in September.
Now Zoo officials are happy to report
that these breeding successes have begun
to bring some real returns. In October a
pair of National-Zoo-born scimitar-horned
oryx were sent to the Haibar Reserve in
Israel, where this species and other rare
mammals native to the Middle Eastern and
North African deserts and desert fringes
have been released to live and propagate
in a suitable environment. Other hoofed
mammals being bred at Haibar include
the dorcas gazelle, the addax, the Nubian
ibex, and the Persian wild ass or onager;
and it was to the same reserve that the
National Zoo recently sent all of its Syrian
brown bears.
In historic times, the scimitar-horned oryx
was abundant on the fringes of deserts
throughout Northern Africa; but by 1850
it was extinct in Egypt, and soon afterwards,
it had disappeared from Morocco, Tunisia,
and Algeria. It is still found in a narrow
band across the southern edge of the Sahara
from Mauritania to the Red Sea. Its numbers
are declining rapidly, however, and the great
herds numbering up to 1,000 which these
8
animals once formed can now no longer be
seen. Only in one small area in northern
Chad is it present in numbers sufficient to
survive long, and even there it is in urgent
need of protection.
The major reason for the decline of the
scimitar-horned oryx throughout its range
has been the destruction of its highly
specialized desert-fringe habitat. This has
been a consequence of overstocking such
regions with domestic animals, which have
destroyed the available vegetation and
thus in effect added whole areas to the
desert. In addition, uncontrolled hunting
has taken a considerable toll.
The remaining scimitar-horned oryx share
their inhospitable homeland with scattered
bands of hardy human nomads. Some of
these have depended on them for centuries
in a manner similar to the way the Indians
of the North American plains depended on
the bixon. They have used the oryx’s meat
for food and obtained a great variety of
other products from it, from spear tips
made from its horns to leather made from
its hide. In the past, such tribes seem to
have performed the function of natural pre-
dators, eliminating the less fit members of
the herds but never overexploiting this
resource on which they depended.
More recently the hunting has begun to
have harmful effects; nomadic tribesmen
have begun to hunt the oryx not only to
meet their own needs but for the profit to
be obtained from the sale of their dried
meat throughout Africa. Moreover, un-
controlled shooting of scimitar-horned oryx
by oil surveyors and military personnel
is believed to be taking a heavy toll.
In a situation such as this, the establish-
ment of protected populations like that
at Haibar is essential, and captive breeding
can provide a valuable source of animals
for reintroduction into the wild in such
reserves when they are established. The
Arabian oryx (Oryx /leucoryx), an even
more endangered relative of the scimitar-
horned oryx, was saved from imminent ex-
tinction only a decade ago by similar methods. -
With the wild population bery small and un-
protected, several captive breeding herds were
established, including one at the Phoenix Zoo
in Arizona. The existence of these captive
herds guarantees the survival of the Arabian
oryx until adequate reserves are established in
its native habitat. In the case of the scimi-
tar-horned oryx, this second step — the
return to the wild — is already beginning,
and the National Zoo is proud to contri-
bute the young male and female “Eric”
and “Margaret’’ that have recently been
sent to Israel.
Dik-Dik Herd Established
The male dik-dik, in foreground, with one of the
four new females (number 9i on map). Notice that
the female’s forehead crest is erected.
For the past several years there has been
another occupant in the Zoo’s red kanga-
roo yard (number 9i on map) besides the
kangaroos — a seldom. noticed male Kirk’s
dik-dik (Rhynchotragus kirkii). Kirk’s dik-
dik is a tiny species of antelope, which
weighs less than a dozen pounds full
grown. A comparison of its size with the
size of some of the larger horned ungulates
(family Bovidae) — for instance the greater
kudu (number 8a on map) or the Cape
buffalo (number 9h on map) — illustrates -
the considerable range of variation with
respect to size among members of this
family.
Last summer the National Zoo placed an
order for two female dik-diks to join its
single male, to be shipped from their
native East Africa. While the two waited
in quarantine in the port of Mombassa,
Kenya, both of them gave birth; and their
calves also proved to be females. Thus,
9
when the new dik-diks arrived in October,
the Zoo’s male acquired a ‘“‘harem”’ of
four. Hopefully with a herd of this size
and new offspring to come, the Zoo’s
dik-diks will not now be as frequently
overlooked by visitors as the shy and incon-
spicuous male was by himself. However,
since these natives of the forest fringes
are mainly nocturnal in the wild, it is
best to look for them in the early morn-
ing or late afternoon.
Adult female dik-diks are frequently
larger than adult males, and one of the
Zoo’s new adult females is larger than the
male, while the other is just about his
size. On arrival, one of the young females
was about the size of the smaller adult
female, while the other was considerably
smaller. It was this smallest female that
emerged first when the dik-dik’s shipping
crate was opened in their new home. The
male came over towards her, and both
were extremely excited. Kirk’s dik-diks
have a crest of reddish fur on the forehead
which they erect when excited, and both
dik-diks erected their crests. The male
marked grasses with special scent glands
located in front of his eyes and attempted
to mate with the young female. Later,
the three other females also emerged from
the crate and scurried around the corner
with their crests erected.
In Kirk’s dik-dik the snout is estended
into a kind of trunk that can be twisted
in any direction to pick up scents. The
diminutive horns appear only in the male.
There are two widely separated popula-
tions of this species in the wild, one in
Angola on the west coast of Africa and
one in Kenya and Tanzania on the east
coast. Both populations are presently endan-
gered in much of their range because of
excessive human predation.
Pygmy Hippo Births
More than 50 pygmy hippos (Choeropsis
liberiensis) have been born at the National
Zoo’s Elephant House (number 17 on map)
since 1925. In addition, in some of these
cases the mothers themselves have been
born at the Zoo. Recently, one more calf
has been added to this number — born to a
Zoo-born mother (‘‘Millie’’) in October.
It’s father was ‘‘Totota,”’ who has sired
approximately half of the pygmy hippos
born here. As has been the custom with
Totota’s offspring, the calf was named for
Pigmy hippo mother and young at the Elephant
House (number 8a on map).
a Greek letter, ‘Psi’, but, since there is only
one more letter left in the Greek alphabet,
it is apparent that Zoo officials will soon
have to adopt a new naming convention.
Millie was born at the National Zoo in 1947
and has given birth to a dozen calves.
Black Rhino Yard Under Construction
The former goat mountain and camel areas
(number 14 on map) are being remodeled
as a new and spacious black rhinoceros
exhibit. The existing stone buildings are
to be retained, but the enclosed areas have
been consolidated and enlarged somewhat
to accomodate up to five black rhinos.
The Zoo’s current pair of adult black.
rhinos (Diceros bicornis) are to share the
new enclosure with the young female born.
to them in 1970, and future offspring
will remain with them. The goat mountain
will also be retained, and African pygmy
goats (Capra hircus) will live there and
share the rhinos’ enclosure.
10
At this writing it is not yet certain when
the rhinos will be moved to their new
home. The construction is expected to be
finished by the first of the year; but,
since it Is considered inadvisable to move
the rhinos in cold weather, moving will
have to wait until a prolonged warm spell
and possibly until spring. The newly avail-
able space at the Elephant House (number
17 on map) will be used for expanded
Indian rhinoceros and African elephant
quarters.
Woolly Monkey Born
Woolly monkey baby clinging to its mother’s back
(number 21 on map).
On September 22, a wooly monkey (Lago-
thrix lagothrica) was born at the Monkey
House (number 27 on map). This birth is
a notable event because, like all too many
New World monkeys, this species has rarely
done well in captivity in the past and has
seldom bred. The Zoo’s breeding pair have
thrived, however; the male has been here
since 1969, and the female since 1964.
They have produced three offspring alto-
gether; but one of these was stillborn, and
one died in an unfortunate accident. Thus
the latest infant, which is to all appearan-
ces a model young primate, represents the
first real breeding success with this species
at the National Zoo and one of a small
number in the world’s zoos.
Although the latest National Zoo birth
was not observed, previous observations
of captive births have given a picture of
woolly monkey birth procedure. The
mother is reported to stand on all fours,
looking around from time to time and
occasionally reaching back to touch the
infant with her hand during the fifteen
minutes of delivery. As soon as it is born,
the infant clings instinctively to its mother’s
fur. She brings it around to the front of
her body and cleans it thoroughly. She
removes the umbilical cord with her teeth,
apparently within few hours after birth.
Little is known of the natural history of
this species, which is found in the densest
forest of the central and upper Amazon
basin and up to altitudes of 7,000 feet in
the Andes. Living in small groups, they
are almost wholly arboreal and make
frequent use of the strongly prehensile
tail in their methodical progress through
the treetops. The spiralled tip of the tail
has a patch of naked skin on its underside
which gives it the sensitivity and grip of
a ‘fifth hand” in taking hold of branches,
and the tail alone can support the entire
weight of the woolly monkey’s body.
Interestingly, the Zoo’s newborn woolly
monkey’s tail is short and sparsely furred
in comparison to the long, woolly tails
of the adults. It is nonetheless an effec-
tive grasping organ, as can be seen when
the baby wraps it tightly around one of
its mother’s thighs as it clings to her
abdomen where her two nipples are located.
At this writing, the infant, whose sex
has not yet been determined, is just begin-
ning to take an interest in its surroundings,
which expresses itself mainly in explora-
tion of the mother’s body. The young
monkey frequently climbs onto her chest,
back, or hip from its usual clinging place
on her abdomen; as yet its interest in the
world beyond its mother seems to be
confined to an occasional reaching out
from her to grasp one of the cage bars.
11
11. Elephant House
12. Water birds (a-e)
13. Hawks and owls (a-c)
1
4. Black Rhinoceros yard
1. Connecticut Avenue pedestrian entrance 15. Small Mammal Building
2. Connecticut Avenue vehicular entrance 16. Lesser Pandas
3. Deer and antelope areas (a-j) 17. Prarie dogs
4. Great Flight Cage 18. Bears and monkeys (a-m)
5. Bird House 19. Reptile House
6. Pheasant and crane line (a-r) 20. Tortoise yard
7. Raptor cages (a-d) 21. Monkey House
8. Delicate-hoofed stock building (a-c) 22. Hardy Animals (a-o)
9. Hardy-hoofed stock complex (a-i) 23. Lion House
10. Panda House (a-c) 24. Komodo Dragon
25. Bears (a-j)
26. Water animals (a-e)
27. Sea Lion pool Telephone
28. Wolves, foxes, and wild dogs (a-l)
29. Lesser Cats Restrooms
é9
ye
wgerey
aes
30. Waterfowl ponds (a-d)
31. Police Station—Restrooms—First Aid
32. Restaurant
33. Picnic Area
34. Window Shop
35. Rock Creek Parkway entrance
36. Friends of the National Zoo Offices
37. FONZ Education, Editorial, and Tour
| Guide Offices
Trackless Train Stops
Parking
vie
R
Ck Cree,
e
22f
2c ;
229
Ay A) Soo, 221,223
o OH 22k
5 221
hey, id 22m
Ny (3
YY 22n
~~ BF 22,
13
.-
-
—-
~ bs
F —
i
\
. th
z ‘
A
= “
os
ad
'
x
cz}
; +
Ae cot
i
.
or
*
wee
*
*
ae
ZCOMAP
. Connecticut Avenue pedestrian entrance
Ma |
SOMNAHSWN=
Connecticut Avenue vehicular entrance
Deer and antelope areas (a-})
. Great Flight Cage
Bird House
Pheasant and crane line (a-r)
. Raptor cages (a-d)
. Delicate-hoofed stock building (a-c)
. Hardy-hoofed stock complex (a-i)
. Panda House (a-c)
. Elephant House
. Water birds (a-e)
. Hawks and owls (a-c)
. Black Rhinoceros yard
. Small Mammal Building
. Lesser Pandas
. Prarie dogs
. Bears and monkeys (a-m)
. Reptile House
. Tortoise yard
. Monkey House
. Hardy Animals (a-o)
. Lion House
. Komodo Dragon
. Bears (a-j)
. Water animals (a-e)
. sea Lion pool
. Wolves, foxes, and wild dogs (a-l)
. Lesser Cats
. Waterfowl ponds (a-d)
. Police Station—Restrooms—F irst Aid
. Restaurant
. Picnic Area
. Window Shop
. Rock Creek Parkway entrance
. Friends of the National Zoo Offices
. FONZ Education, Editorial, and Tour
Guide Offices
yore)
Telephone
Restrooms
Trackless Train Stops
Parking
Male Tiger on Breeding Loan
Visitors to the Lion House (number 23 on
map) will notice a new tiger (Panthera
tigris) on exhibit there. This normally
colored male is here on a six-month breed-
ing loan, during which time Zoo officials
hope to breed him both with Mohini, the
National Zoo’s famous white tigress, and
i
Scarlet Ibis Chick Hatched
On September 26th a single egg was hatched
by a pair of scarlet ibis in their spacious cage
at Bird House (number 5 on map). At first
the hatchling was almost constantly brooded
by one of its parents and could rarely be
seen by Zoo visitors. But after it reached the
age of about a week, it was frequently visible,
standing up in the nest and cheeping strenu-
ously or soliciting food from a parent by
tapping vigorously near the tip of the adult
bird’s long, curved bill. When the nestling
persisted in its begging, the adult would open
its bill wide, and the nestling would reach
deep into its throat pouch where the food
was stored.
In appearance the scarlet ibis chick was very
different from its graceful, brilliantly
colored parents. Its downy plumage was
with her normally colored daughter,
Kesari. It is hoped that by siring off-
spring here the new tiger will add fresh
genes to the National Zoo’s Bengal tiger
gene pool. The new male (nicknamed _
“Poona’’) was born at the Seattle Zoo in
1962 and has lived at Chicago’s Brook-
field Zoo since 1963.
The Zoo's scarlet ibis breeding pair on their nest
at the Bird House (number 5 on map).
black over most of its body and dirty-white
on the underside. Its bill was much shorter
in comparison to its body size than the
adult’s and was striped with yellow.
The young ibis will change in appearance
very gradually, reaching fully adult plumage
only after three or four years. After its
first plumage change, which is expected to
take place when the chick is about two
months old, it will be grayish brown on its
wings and back and white on its underside.
14
At the age of six months, it will begin to
show a few pale orange feathers on the wings;
and its underparts, lower back, and thighs
will have a reddish wash.
Scarlet ibises are native to the northeast
coast of South America from Venezuela to
eastern Brazil, though occasional birds may
wander as far away as Jamaica and, according
to some reports from the last century, Georgia
and Florida. They nest along the seacoast in
the tops of trees, bushes, or canebrake in
colonies that may number in the thousands.
One observer reported that in such a colony
Prehensile-Tailed Skinks
Four prehensile-tailed skinks are new on
exhibit at the Reptile House (number 19 on
map). These unusual lizards, which are
native to the Solomon Islands in the South
Pacific, are the largest of the world’s more
than 600 species of skinks. Some specimens
have measured as much as two feet in length.
Many members of the skink family—which is
represented on every continent and on many
“the bended tops of the canes were covered
by half-grown birds in their black plumage,
interspersed with many in all the brilliance of
age.’’ After breeding season, the young birds
have been reported to form separate flocks
that travel and feed apart from the adults.
But a little later the flocks that are seen
searching for small fish, mollusks, and crus-
taceans on beaches or exposed mudflats often
include birds in full adult plumage, birds in
the brown juvenile plumage, and birds in
every transitional stage between the two.
nsile-tailed skinks.
oceanic islands—can readily climb trees and
bushes, but the prehensile-tailed skink is the
only one that has evolved a really notable
adaptation for arboreal life. This is its strong
tail, which can be wrapped around a branch
to aid the lizard when it is climbing.
15
Madagascan Day Geckos
Four members of another arboreal lizard
species, the Madagascan day gecko (Phe/suma
madagensis) have also recently been acquired.
These small lizards are attractively colored,
with bright green backs and red spots near
the head. As in other arboreal geckos, the
wide circular pads on their toes have an
amazing adhesive power that enables the
lizards to cling to surfaces as smooth as a
sheet of glass.
The nature of the gecko’s grip was a mystery
for a long time, but recently electron micro-
scopes have revealed that the gecko’s toes
are covered with tiny hairlike bristles each of
which has a suction cup on its tip. These
bristles are arranged in ridges along the gecko’s
toes, and the toes can be bent backwards to
release them from a surface.
The Madagascan day gecko is named for the
fact that, unlike most geckos, it is active
during the day. The tokay gecko (Gekko
gecko) of Malaysia in cage C-11 is a nocturnal
gecko also on exhibit at the Reptile House. A
third gecko species at the National Zoo is the
leopard gecko (Eublepharis macularis); the
pair of these geckos that are on exhibit in
cage A-1 have bred, and several of their eggs
were hatched this past autumn. Of the new
day geckos, two are males, and two are
females; so Reptile House officials hope to
be able to repeat their breeding success with
the new species as well.
Amphiumas—Unusual Salamanders
A recent addition to the collection of amphi-
bians at the Reptile House (number 19 on
map) is a pair of two-toed amphiumas
(Amphiuma means) in section F. Known
popularly as congo eels or ‘‘conger eels,”
these aquatic salamanders are not true eels
(which are fish) but members of an ancient
family of amphibians that has existed at
least since the Cretaceous Period some 120
million years ago. With their long, cylindrical
black bodies, amphiumas are somewhat eel-
like in appearance, but their tiny, useless
front and back legs and their possession of
lungs reveal their amphibian ancestry.
Amphiumas are found only in the south-
eastern United States as far west as Louisiana
and Missouri. Typically they inhabit still or
slow-moving water in ponds, swamps, ditches,
and streams, but they sometimes move over-
land from one body of water to another,
particularly on rainy nights. They frequently
burrow into mud or heaps of debris at the
bottom or side of a pool and wait in ambush
there for a crayfish, worm, small fish, or
other suitable prey item to pass.
Amphiumas breed in the water. In most
salamanders the male actively courts the
female; but in the amphiuma, a number of
females will compete for the attentions of
one male by rubbing his body with their
snouts. The eggs, which form a long string
numbering about 150 eggs, are laid beneath
logs in shallow water. They are apparently
guarded by the female. Frequently the
shallow water in which the eggs are laid dries
up before they hatch; so the two-and-a-half
to three-inch-long larvae, which are brown
with white dots and have white gills, must
find their way to the water by themselves.
16
17
Visitors to the indoor flight room at the Bird
House (number 5 on map) will have noticed
the active and elegant birds known as hoopoes;
with their prominent crests, their rich fawn-
colored heads and shoulders, and their black-
and-white-striped wings, the eight hoopoes
there are not easily overlooked. Particularly
in the fall and winter months, when human
traffic through the room Is reduced, they can
often be found walking along the floor,
probing for food with their long, thin, down-
curved bills in the crevices of the retaining
wall that surrounds the planted parts of the
room. At other times they can often be seen
feeding in the soil, frantically flicking up dirt
with the tip of the bill or thrusting it almost
full-length into the soft earth.
As the hoopoe feeds, the fan-like crest that
forms its most distinctive feature is frequently
in motion. When the bird is unexcited, the
crest is usually flattened against the head;
the feathers at the front of the crest are
shorter than those at the back of the crest, so
that when it is collapsed the black dots near
the top of each feather coalesce to form three
irregular bands of black across the top of the
flattened crest. The hoopoe opens out its
crest in response to any stimulus that excites
its alarm or interest. When one of the hoopoes
in the indoor flight room expands its crest
fully, it may be a sign that the bird is suffi-
ciently alarmed to abandon its search for
food temporarily and take off for the protec-
tion of the trees or cliffs.
Hoopoes are Old World birds, found in much
of Europe, Asia, and Africa. The single species,
which is divided into about eight geographic
races or subspecies, has no close relatives and
is usually placed by ornithologists in a family
all by itself. But this unique species has suc-
ceeded in adapting to a great variety of
climates and habitats, from the steppes of
Central Asia to the forests of Ceylon in the
east and from the evergreen forests of France
and Germany to the South African veldt in
the west.
Since it inhabits so many different parts of
the world, including many where the human
population is dense and has been relatively
dense for a long time, it is not surprising that
the hoopoe has come to play a role in the
folklore of many of the peoples with whom
it has come in contact. The ancient Egyptians
held the hoopoe sacred, as they did so many
animal species; the hoopoe appears in Egyptian
art as early as 1900 B.C. and was apparently
the basis for the hieroglyph for “gratitude.” .
Centuries later, Medieval European preachers
and religious writers expanded on this latter
fact, inventing many edifying tales of the
hoopoe’s supposed filial gratitude. The
ancient Greeks were likewise fascinated by
hoopoes, and one of them appears as a
character in the comedy The Birds by the
Fifth Century B.C. Athenian Aristophanes.
In that play, Aristophanes makes what seems
to be the first known attempt to reproduce
a bird song by phonetic means, when he
elaborately renders the male hoopoe’s
territorial advertising call: “Epopoi—popoi—
epopopoi—popoi—io—io—io.”
It is this unforgettable call, of course, that
has given the hoopoe its name in English
and in virtually every other language that has
a term for it. The Romans called it upupa,
and the Greeks epops; and both of these
terms seem to be imitative of the male’s
call. Nowadays, these two ancient names
taken together form the hoopoe’s scientific
name, Upupa epops.
As those in the indoor flight room illustrate,
hoopoes do most of their feeding on the
ground. The hoopoe’s feet are excellently
adapted for terrestrial travel, although they
serve equally well for perching in trees; but
the long bill is the most obvious adaptation
for this bird’s feeding habits. Open slightly at
the tip, the bill is used to probe into the
ground for insects and insect larvae dwelling
just below the surface of the soil. (This
methodical probing is doubtless the source
of the belief current among Arab peoples
that the hoopoe can divine for water under-
ground.) Hoopoes also catch any insects they
come across on the surface of the soil or
around the roots of the grasses that are
usually found in the open areas where the
birds feed. Occasionally, too, a hoopoe will
fly up from the ground a short distance to
capture a particularly enticing insect on the
wing.
On catching a prey item, the hoopoe first
seizes It firmly in the tip of its bill, killing the
insect. Then it tosses the insect back, opening
its bill wide at the same time so that the
insect lands in its mouth. Sometimes when it
has captured a larger than ordinary insect, the
hoopoe will actually toss the morsel up into
the air a short distance and open its gape wide
in order to swallow it. In such cases there
can be unexpected complications. A South
African naturalist noticed that a group of
large, crow-like scavenging birds known as
fork-tailed drongos (Dicrurus adsimilis) often
waited on a fence beside a field while hoopoes
fed there; sometimes a drongo would swoop
down and steal an insect from a hoopoe just
18
as the hoopoe was tossing it back into its
mouth.
The hoopoe’s feeding habits have influenced
its choice of habitat throughout its vast
range; it needs patches of open country in
which to forage. Hoopoes also prefer to
nest in holes in trees and in the non-breeding
season to roost at night in trees, and these
requirements usually limit their numbers in
areas where trees are absent or nearly absent.
Typically, hoopoes are found in sunny forest
with open glades, on the edges of forests, or
in areas under human cultivation where trees
are present in small groves or at roadsides.
Their readiness to dwell in proximity to man,
benefiting him considerably by providing a
check on insect populations, has doubtless
been one of the reasons why they have played
such a prominent role in his folklore over the
The distribution of the hoopoe (Upupa epops).
centuries. Now, unfortunately they are
becoming scarce in agricultural regions in
Western Europe and are withdrawing to the
few remaining forests. Folklorists have
already begun to notice that the hoopoe is
dying out of folk belief there or that stories
once told of the hoopoe are now attributed
to other birds; too few people even in
agricultural regions know what a hoopoe
looks like.
The hoopoes of Europe and those that inhabit
similar northerly latitudes in the Asiatic
U.S.S.R., China, and Korea are migratory in
winter. The majority of those migrating from
Europe cross the Mediterranean and pass
through Egypt, following the fertile Nile
Valley to Subsaharan Africa. Northern
Asiatic hoopoes winter in the Southern parts
of the continent from India to Malaya. In
19
both of these areas they share their range with
resident hoopoe populations. Some of these
southerly hoopoes may engage in brief
migrations outside of the breeding season;
hoopoes of the Ceylonese race in Kerala in
southern India, for instance, are reported to
make an annual altitudinal migration, moving
up mountain sides to a height of about 5,000
feet during the hot weather.
With these annual shifts in population, it is
to be expected that hoopoes breed at different
times of the year in different parts of the
world. European and North Asian hoopoes
return in March and April, and evidently the
first breeding activities—occupation of terri-
tories by males and their courtship of females
—begin soon afterwards. The male’s “‘hoop-
hoop”’ call which he uses to warn other males
away from his territory during breeding
season Is heard night and day until late June
and early July, when eggs are laid. In Egypt,
the resident hoopoes are already nesting in
February and March when the European
hoopoes pass by on their migratory flights
to the North.
In South Africa, where hoopoes are also
resident year-round, nesting begins in August
and September and continues until December;
though this is much later in the year than their
relatives nest further north, these Southern
Hemisphere hoopoes have not so completely
altered their breeding cycle that they breed in
their own summer from December to March.
Interestingly, the Ceylonese hoopoe’s breeding
season differs from that of more northerly
populations by being earlier in the year—
from February to April—rather than later
like the South African hoopoe’s.
It has been in South Africa that the best field
work to date on hoopoes has been done.
There the beginning of the breeding season
is marked by an interesting sequence of
behavior. Around middle or late March
hoopoes, which have been solitary for the
preceding two-and-a-half months, begin
feeding in groups numbering as many as
nine birds. Both sexes are represented in
these feeding parties, and all of the birds
are at first quite tolerant of each other,
although at night when they retire to their
roosting trees, there is never more than one
hoopoe per tree. By the middle of April,
however, males begin to show occasional
outbursts of aggression towards each other
and towards females; this is the beginning of
a gradual process leading to the formation
of breeding pairs and the eventual break-up
of the flock.
As the flock is feeding silently and peacefully
on the ground together, one male will suddenly
ruffle his feathers, open his wings, and rush at
another male, making a distinctive harsh
whimpering sound as he does so. The other
male, remaining silent, will run away, and
the first male will continue to pursue him.
The pursued male may take flight, and his
pursuer may then either abandon the chase
or continue to pursue him in the air. If the
second male lands in a tree, the pursuer may
follow and chase him from bough to bough.
Eventually, however, the aggressor seems to
tire of the chase, and the two birds resume
feeding with the others as if nothing had
happened.
Later, in June and July, the pattern of these
aggressive episodes alters, and a male that is
being attacked by another will resist. When
this happens, the males face off, erecting
their crests and bobbing their heads, and
then grab hold of each other’s beaks. Beaks
locked, they flutter up into the air eight or
ten feet before letting go and falling back to
the ground. This ritualized combat may be
repeated two or three times, while the other
members of the flock continue feeding with
apparent indifference. Similar bill-to-bill
combats may occasionally take place among
females during the same period.
Males begin to chase females at the same
time that they begin to chase other males,
but the male’s behavior when pursuing a
female is different and markedly less aggres-
sive. Often he will pause, making a rattling
noise and then two or three very melodious,
woodwind-like notes. These notes seem to be
very subdued versions of the loud whoops
which the male makes later when the breeding
season is in full swing and he is defending a
territory. By June a male and female can
often be seen feeding together apart from the
main flock. As they do so, the male will
sometimes catch an insect and run over and
present it to the female.
The feeding of the female by the male plays
an important role in hoopoe courtship, as
analogous rituals do in many other avian
species. It seems to function both to help
form and, later, to cement the pair bond
between the two birds. The male’s first
attempts at courtship feeding occur before
pairs have begun to separate from the flock.
At this time the female’s response to his
offers of food indicates to him whether or
not she is ready to form a pair bond.
Like other birds, hoopoes undergo an annual
cycle of hormonal changes corresponding to
their annual breeding cycle. While this cycle
seems to be at least partly influenced by
20
external factors such as temperature and day-
length, it does not proceed at exactly the
same rate in any two birds. Thus, as breeding
season approaches, one bird may be ready to
breed somewhat earlier than another. In the
South African Hoopoe and probably in all
hoopoes, males generally enter breeding con-
dition before females. They show aggression
towards each other and towards females before
females begin to quarrel among themselves or
to be responsive to the attentions of males.
In addition, different females come into
breeding condition at different rates. Thus
the female’s response to an attempt at court-
ship feeding can indicate to the male her
state of breeding readiness. Then, if she is
unresponsive, he may take his insect and
offer it to another female; if she accepts, it
is a sign that she has entered the stage of her
annual cycle where she is ready to begin to
form a pair bond.
After the formation of the pair bond, con-
tinued courtship feeding and other courtship
activities can function to synchronize the
breeding rhythms of the two birds and ensure
that both are ready to establish a territory,
copulate, and build a nest at the same time.
Also, since in courtship feeding the female is
placed in the same dependent relation towards
the male as she once had towards her parents,
it serves to enhance his dominance over her.
Still more important, perhaps, in the case of
the hoopoe is the fact that courtship feeding
is a prelude to the time when the female is
incubating the eggs and brooding the young;
for at that time she will depend to a great
extent on food brought her by the male.
Eventually the South African hoopoe’s pre-
breeding flock is broken apart by the increas-
ing aggressiveness of the males, and each pair
takes up residence at a nest site, evidently
chosen by the male. This happens soon after
the males begin to make the famous “hoop-
hoop’’ call. At first one male will fly up into
a tree, begin giving this call, and then return
to the ground and furiously attack other
males in the flock. Finally, the feeding
ground on which the flock has gathered day
after day is divided up into private territories
by a few of the males, each of which defends
his portion by giving the “hoop-hoop”’ call
from a prominent perch. Any surplus males
must go elsewhere in search of territories of
their own.
The territory that the male hoopoe defends
is an open grassy area suitable for feeding. It
is near the site of his nest but not necessarily
right next to it. It is this feeding area that he
uses his territorial calling to defend and not
the area around the nest; by contrast in some
other avian species breeding males defend
their nests and the areas around them but
intermingle freely on feeding grounds. The
importance of the feeding ground to the male
hoopoe is doubtless based on the fact that he
will need a nearby and steady source of insect
food to feed the female while she is incubating
the eggs and to feed the young when they
hatch.
It has apparently not yet been determined
how closely pair formation in migratory
hoopoe populations follows the pattern that
has been described for the South African
race. It is not known, for instance, whether
migratory hoopoes returning to Europe and
northern Asia ever feed together in flocks
before they pair up for breeding season. It
is usually said that hoopoes return to Europe
singly or in pairs, but they are known to
sometimes return in groups of eight to ten
birds and groups of as many as four have
been seen soon after the hoopoes have
returned and before they have started
breeding. It is quite possible, however, that
migratory hoopoes feed together in flocks
in their winter homes before migrating and
that pair-formation takes place there in a
manner similar to the way in which it takes
place in the flocks of the South African
hoopoe. This would perhaps explain the
many reports of migratory hoopoes returning
in pairs, which in turn seem to be the basis
for the common but unproved assertion that
in Europe—as does not seem to be the case
in South Africa—hoopoes mate for life.
The Zoo’s hoopoes are from India, perhaps
from a non-migratory population; and it is a
matter of great interest that they have been
observed this past fall engaging in behavior
similar to that described for South African
hoopoes in the pre-breeding flock. (That the
Zoo’s hoopoes do not belong to the South
African race can easily be determined, since
they possess a white band across the pri-
maries, the large front feathers of the wing,
which is lacking in the South African form.)
On repeated occasions one hoopoe was seen
chasing another in the way males chase males
and other females in pre-breeding flocks in
South Africa. A few times, too, the pursuing
bird showed behavior exactly like that
described for a male pursuing a female when,
either silent or making a harsh whimpering
sound as he chased her, he paused to cackle
and then produce two or three soft ‘‘hoop”’
notes.
The typical hoopoe nesting site is a hollow
tree, but hoopoes may occasionally employ
21
taphtsit
Hy
iit
ilaetesptne
Sift
Hie
i
i
i
i
i
sie
ey
i
He HH
He Hi
Hie
Hs
i
a
a
Bs
fis
ti
thts
i
fi
iii
tite
Hf
if
Hi
irr
Hie
ij
i
i
ie
fe
Hil
eesperpeitta
i
Bf
Ht
Be
Hy
i
dette
tit
Hi
HE
fais8
Hi
Hite
Hitt
He
if
Ht Hie
Hi
Hise
j
inattat:
Ht
i
Heh
He
Hy}
Hitt
itis
Hite
His
ve
i
tinstaftetose
li
Hii
ie y ; i
et
Gti
We
bite
Hi
i
Hi
ities Hiieh
i
Hite
bi
lived
ve
His
Hs
Hts
iid
Hie A
eitti!
bite
Ha
ite
i
if
biiatt
GH iB
Hs if
sit ii)
Hy
He
Hie
Hitt
th
Hi
i Hy
HAT
iD
}
i
i Hi
seis
isihaft ff xf ff
eh sits
Hi
isin
i
Hi i
He
i estas
if i
i
ff i PH
is Hi
i i iif. Hi Ht
it
He
i
beth
i
He
en
Hi
Hii
a
es
Ee
i}
Hes
i
i
He
Pin
ee
i
j
rahi fis
j
ee
i
ihe
ae
lata iii
i i
His fii
bifiaitth
He
Hes
it,
Hf
iis
Hi
if
Hy
i
He ie
i Hise
hit Hae
fit
iis
Bi
riley
fey
able
i
Hetty Hey
Mihai}
eh
sith
ite He
tif
istintents
i}
bi
i
Mee
tis Line
He
ths
Hf
Fie
i
iif
fietie
iat
fe
i
i
bit
i
i
Hi
iit
He
ites
f
Hy
ti
Hie
He
it
biti
i
i
iy
if
Hips
a
Hy
|
ite
ts
Heats
iy
Hs
Pa
hi
Ri
i
iit
it i i
HH i i Hy
7
by
a
i EA
i Hi He f
sh iii
if He if
Hie iH i
Biss Lite ea ay
‘ i
Hi
bi i
ae
site
ite
i
ah:
Hi
ii
i
iasestesitee
f
f
Hh
tte
iy
esti
Hi
Hh
7
rei
Hi
ne
i,
Hh:
Hh
eee
Hee
Pe
Hie
i
Hf
fi;
Hi
Hae
He
ti
ie
bit
tie
bi
i
Hil
He
His
Maiti
Hi
iia
is
i
ij
i
i
/
i
fi
ih
Mf
ist Hi
i
i
i
i
He He
Hh Ts
Be i
Bi
ii
Hi
bitte
ti
iis
ii
He
a
L He
iit
Yi
fe
Hii
ih
if
bist
ti
iste;
Cia
Hitiste Hebi
He
ii
Hinze
Hi
tri
i
_
ves ii
He Hit
riety oi biirh
f
tif iris
Hie
HH
Bile
sitet
Hitt}
He
i
his
Lait
it
tess
aoa
byt
Hi
|
iif
Hh
Ui
Hf
Hi
i
ii
Hifi
i
if
HH
Lftists
i
a variety of other hollows, both natural and
man-made. In Egypt, they have been seen
nesting in crevices in the stone faces of the
pyramids, while on the almost treeless
steppes of the central U.S.S.R. they nest in
the eaves of buildings and even in sandy
depressions in the ground or in the abandoned
burrows of other animals. The nest holes in
the faces of the artificial cliffs in the National
Zoo’s Indoor Flight Room have provided
ideal hoopoe nesting sites in the past.
Some hoopoes place a simple pile of grasses
or leaves in the nesting cavity, while others
lay their four to seven eggs on the bare floor
of the nest hole. The mother hoopoe does all
the incubating, remaining continually on the
nest except for brief periods of about half an
hour in the morning or evening when she
emerges to feed on her own. Off and on
during the rest of the day the male brings her
food. This pattern continues after the eggs
hatch, and the female apparently eats some
of the food the male brings and passes some
on to her young. In the nestling hoopoe,
which has fluffy pinkish-red plumage, the
bill is relatively shorter than it is in the adult.
The bill opening is lined with white, while the
inside of the mouth is red; thus the nestling’s
wide-open mouth forms an excellent ‘‘target”’
for the mother directing her bill towards it
in the darkness of the nest hole.
When the nest is disturbed, both the mother
and her young make a violent hiss, which,
issuing from a darkened cavity, must fre-
quently be taken for the sound of a snake.
The nestling’s preen gland secretes a strong-
smelling substance when frightened that
functions as an additional deterrent against
mammalian predators. How effective it Is
can be illustrated by the success it has had in
deceiving the most dangerous of all mam-
malian predators, man. Men have advanced
many different theories for thousands of
years to explain the odor they encountered
on disturbing hoopoes’ nests, although they
do not seem to have arrived at the correct
explanation before our own century. The
explanations they have invented, however,
have succeeded in so repelling their own
human inventors that they have by and see
left hoopoes’ nests alone.
The most common belief has been that hoopoe
parents are extremely negligent about nest
sanitation. Thus Aristophanes’ hoopoe makes.
Sanitation than some other cavity-nesting
birds, the source of the odor intruders on
hoopoes’ nests find is not uncleanliness but
the nestlings’ protective secretion. But its
supposed filthy habits were probably the
reason why the hoopoe—mistranslated “‘lap-
wing” in the Authorized Version—was among
the animals listed as forbidden foods in the
Bible. And even today it is said that in Europe
hoopoes are never eaten in areas where they
nest, whereas in areas where they only pass as
migrants they are frequently killed for food.
Between August of 1970 and March of 1972,
a single pair of hoopoes in the National Zoo’s
indoor flight room hatched and raised five
clutches. Unfortunately, one member of the
pair was later killed by an unexpected preda-
tor, a bald-headed rock-fowl (Picathartes
gymnocephalus), all members of that species
were immediately moved from the indoor
flight room and placed in cage 8 with large
birds they cannot harm. However, since
hoopoes are reportedly able to breed in the
next breeding season after they hatch and
since there are abundant Zoo-born hoopoes
in the indoor flight room, it is possible that
there will be breeding again this winter or, if
not then, by next summer.
Hopeful signs have been observed repeatedly
this fall, apparently indicating that there is
at least one male approaching breeding con-
dition among the Zoo’s eight hoopoes. It is
possible that not only one male but also one
or two others could form breeding pairs and
nest in the indoor flight room during the next
few months. Since territoriality in this species
seems to be largely dependent on food supply
and there is abundant food for all, as many as
two or even three individual feeding terri-
tories could conceivably be established and
defended there. So, if all goes well this
winter, visitors can look for increased activity
on the part of both males and females—
including bill-to-bill combats and courtship
feeding—leading up to a time when the
hoopoe’s famous call is heard in the Bird
House once again.
a point of apologizing for the condition of his » 1
home. This sort of explanation took another
form according to which the hoopoe was the
original ‘“‘ill bird that fouls its own nest.” In
fact, while the hoopoe is less fastidious about
23