Volume 3, Number 6 November/December 1974 | Price .50
Published by the Friends of the National Zoo
Published by
Friends of the National Zoo
National Zoological Park
Washington, D.C. 20009
Phones:
Executive Director and membership:
232-7700
Education and Volunteer Offices:
232-7703
Guided tours: 232-7703
Train tours: 232-7704
Window Shop: 232-7705
FONZ Board of Directors 1974-75
Arthur Arundel, President
Montgomery S. Bradley, First Vice President
Lavell Merritt, Second Vice President
Stephen Hosmer, /reasurer
Joan L. Jewett, Secretary
Ruth N. Nelson
William N. Olinge:
John B. Oliver
Peter C. Andrews
Theodore Babbitt
John S. Brown
Timothy V.A. Dillon Julie Pineau
Ronald Field Nancy Porter
M. Anthony Gould Ted Rivinus
Donna K. Grosvenor Whayne Quin
Nella Manes Lee Talbot
Gerald G. Wagner
Rosa M. Walker
Cecil McLelland
Shirley J. McNair
Executive Director
Sabin Robbins
Writer: Austin Hughes
Photograph on page 21 by Sabin Robbins, all
others by Ray Fass. Drawing on page 5 by
Warren Cutler, on page 12 by Susan Genovesi
Hughes.
Production: Monica Johansen
The meerkat is an intriguing social mam-
mal. A colony has recently been placed on
exhibit at the Small Mammal House (see
page 9).
THE ZOOGOER is published bi-monthly
and copyrighted © by Friends of the National
Zoo, c/o National Zoological Park, Washington,
D.C. 20009, second-class mailing permit
approved at Washington, D.C. Rate in the
United States $3 a year (of annual dues).
CONTENTS
Quoth the Raven .
: ZooNews
11 Down the Hatch
17. BookNews
20 FONZNews
Zoo Staff
r. Theodore H. Reed, Director
i Edward Kohn, Deputy Director
Mr. Warren J. Iliff, Assistant Director
Mr. John Perry, Assistant Director
Mr. Jaren Horsley, General Curator
Mr. Harold Egoscue, Curator (Mammals)
Mr. William Xanten, Curator (Mammals)
Mr, Guy Greenwell, Curator (Birds)
Mr, Larry Collins, Associate Curator (Mammals)
Mr. Miles Roberts, Assistant Curator (Mammals)
Mr. Michael Davenport, Assistant Curator
(Reptiles)
Dr. Clinton Gray, Veterinarian
Dr. Mitchell Bush, Veterinarian
Dr. John Eisenberg, Resident Scientist
Dr. Helmut Buechner, Senior Ecologist
Dr. Devra Kleiman, Reproductive Zoologist
Mr. Norm Melun, Architect
Mr. Emanuel Petrella, Chief, Buildings & Grounds
For information concerning the collection call
381-7283 or 381-7284.
For general visitor information call 381-7235.
Friends
of
the
National
The Friends of the National Zoo is a non-profit
organization of individuals and families who
frequently visit the National Zoo and who are
interested in supporting its growth and develop-
ment, particularly in the areas of education,
conservation, and scientific research.
As members of the Friends, you and your family
will be given benefits that will make your zoo-
going more enjoyable and educational.
For more information and a membership appli-
cation, please call 232-7700.
s Fitba
There used to be a raven behind the
Bird House that regularly traded coos
with the lace-necked doves in the Great
Flight Cage. He also did a pretty good
imitation of one of the Indian hill
mynah’s most common calls. Nobody
taught him; he just seemed to pick it
up. Like a number of other bird spe-
cies, the raven is a natural mimic. He
has a _ built-in talent for repeating
sounds he hears.
Pet ravens have been taught to repeat
human speech, as have their relatives
the crows, jackdaws, and magpies. Ed-
gar Allen Poe must have had such a
raven in mind when writing his famous
poem. Members of this group of birds
—the crow family—combine consider-
able intelligence (for birds at least)
with a vocal apparatus capable of
reproducing the relatively deep, un-
musical tones of the human voice.
Trained magpies were favorites of the
ancient Romans, and they inspired
some amusing passages in the Natural
History of the learned but credulous
Roman author Pliny. Pliny fell into the
fallacy of believing that magpies under-
stood the words they imitated. “They
become fond of uttering certain
words,” he pontificated. “They not
only learn them but come to love them
and secretly ponder them with careful
reflection, not concealing their engross-
ment.’’ Pliny does not reveal which
words these were. Further, Pliny at-
tributed to the trained magpie a touchy
pride in his abilities: “‘It is an estab-
lished fact that if any word is too hard
for them to repeat, this causes their
death.”’
Today members of the parrot family
are much better known as “‘talkers”’
than members of the crow family.
Though world-wide trade in parrots
began only in the Renaissance, parrots
have “‘talked” from time immemorial
in their native countries. The great
German naturalist Alexander von Hum-
boldt, traveling in Brazil in the Eigh-
teenth Century, heard a parrot speak a
dead language. All the members of the
tribe that trained the parrot had died
out during the bird’s long lifetime.
But, in the faithful reproduction of the
human vowel sounds, the Indian _ hill
mynah is the champion talker of all.
This bird belongs to still a third family
—the starling family. Interestingly, the
common starling has also proved a
fairly good talker. Folk wisdom used to
hold that if you split a_ starling’s
tongue, he will learn to talk. The same
procedure was also used on magpies,
crows, and ravens. In fact this rather
barbarous practice in no way Improves
‘the bird’s mimetic abilities and proba-
bly lessen them.
Brian Bertram recently studied the
behavior of the hill mynah in India and
gained some insight into the source of
its powers as a mimic. In the wild, a
young hill mynah must learn a wide
repertory of calls. It learns them by:
listening to its elders. A good talker is a
bird taken from the wild before he has
learned much of a natural call-reper-
tory. And, like the other birds that can
imitate human speech, the mynah’s
natural voice ts low.
Not all birds have to learn their calls or
songs. A European wheatear, hand-
raised in isolation from any others of
its species, will grow up to sing the
wheatear’s typical song pretty much
note perfect. In this species, the song is
said to be innate or instinctive. In some
other species it is partly innate and
The mocking bird, the best natural mimic in North America.
partly learned. The basic pattern may learned. In some species, including the
be innate, but embellishments are hill mynah, it seems that nearly all
adult vocalizations have to be learned.
This probably holds true for all good
talkers.
Every zoo has its favorite talking-bird
story. The National Zoo’s concerns
another member of the starling-mynah
family, a Javanese mynah. The source
is the late Director William Mann, in his
book Wild Animals In and Out of the
Zoo. Sometime in the 1930’s General
av... bord. then, Director of the
Budget, attended a fair at which each
branch of the Smithsonian Institution
had an exhibit. The Zoo was then, as
always, pressed for funds; and, as Lord
toured the Zoo exhibit, he was pur-
posely brought to a halt before the
carefully schooled mynah’s cage. “How
about the appropriation?” the bird
said, as expected. “That’s imperti-
nent,” replied the General. ‘So’s your
old man,” said the bird. But the. Zoo
got $30,000.
In the Seventeenth and Eighteenth
Centuries it was also fashionable to
teach cage birds to sing. The owner
would repeat a breif melody ona flute
or on a special high-pitched wind
instrument known as a “‘bird-flageolet”’
until his pet picked it up. The starling
was evidently the only species to be
trained both as a ‘‘talker” and a “sing-
er’’—a tribute to its great range in pitch
and its well-known ability to produce
both very musical whistles and harsh
low-pitched croaks. Mozart at one
point owned a “singing” starling. But
mostly the singers were finches or
house sparrows, species whose natural
whistling tones were already rather
flute-like.
One author tells of a woman in our
own century who taught a bullfinch to
whistle ‘‘God Save the King.” A
canary, kept in an adjoining room, a
year later picked up the tune from the
bullfinch. Sometimes, if the bullfinch
paused too long between the third and
fourth lines of the song, the canary
would. take over and complete the
stanza.
Besides those trained by man, there are
other mimics in the bird world—species
‘that incorporate them in their own
songs. Among North American birds,
the mockingbird is the best known
mimic of this sort. His relatives, the
catbird and the brown thrasher, evi-
dently engage in a little song-stealing
too. Appropriately, the family to
which the three species belong is
known as Mimidae. The mockingbird
himself is given the scientific name
Mimus polyglottus in honor of his
supremacy among mimics on this con-
tinent. Actually, well-traveled ornitho-
logists report that yet another member
of the family, a South American spe-
cies known as the white-banded mock-
ingbird, is even more proficient. This
bird is native to the temperate part of
South America and migrates north to
winter in the tropics. When he returns
south each spring, he reportedly brings
with him a whole new collection of
songs he has picked up in the exotic
lands visited on his travels. Our mock-
ingbirds rarely migrate; so perhaps they
The African gray parrot is reputed to the best “talker” among parrots.
can be excused for having more limited
repertories.
One has to be careful when dealing
with the question of bird mimicry. A
lot of mimicry exists only in the ear of
the listener. And there are tall tales
scattered throughout the extensive
literature that has accumulated on the
subject. For example, one enthusiastic
writer early in this century opined that
an average mockingbird might give his
impression of some 50 other species
over the course of an hour’s singing. In
fact, the mockingbird’s song is almost
always 90% pure mockingbird. The
genuine borrowings are rare.
Like species that can be taught to talk
or sing by man, good natural mimics
are species that must learn their songs.
The mockingbird learns most of his
varied song, and most of it he learns
from other mockingbirds. He is a born
imitator because he has to be to learn
his song. But some mockingbirds are
more inveterate mimics than is strictly
necessary. From time to time such a
bird picks up a phrase or two from
another species. Other mockingbirds
pick it up from him. It becomes part of
the local mockingbird dialect, so to
speak.
However, if a mockingbird is in a mood
to learn a song and there are no other
mockingbirds to learn from, he takes
what he can get. There is the famous
case Of a mockingbird that at a young
age somehow strayed out of the spe-
cles’ natural range and found himself
all alone in Massachusetts. His song
consisted almost entirely of borrowings
from other species. With no mother
tongue to draw on, this mocker was a
“polyglot” indeed.
The Indian hill mynah combines great learning ability with a low-pitched voice. No other bird is so suc-
cessful at accurately reproducing human speech.
A SONENS
Meerkat Exhibit Opens
Visitors to South Africa have fre-
quently been amused by the behavior
of an unexpectedly intelligent animal
known as the suricate or meerkat (Suri-
cata suricatta). The name ‘‘meerkat”’
means “‘little cat’ in Afrikaans—a
zoologically inaccurate but understand-
able appellation. The meerkat is a kind
of mongoose and is a highly social
species. A tribe of meerkats foraging
over the savannah presents an engaging
sight. They overturn stones and sticks
in search of insect tidbits, all the while
keeping up an almost continuous “‘con-
versation”’ of calls.
A new exhibit at the National Zoo’s
Small Mammal House contains a little
tribe of meerkats, a female and four
males, and they show some of the
complex social interaction that makes
these animals so interesting to watch in
the wild.
A wild meerkat group living in the
open savannah has a communal burrow
system to which it retires at night.
When a group of meerkats is resting in
the vicinity of its burrow system, one
may stand guard, sitting up on its
haunches. At the sight of an enemy—a
hawk, for instance—the sentinel will
give an alarm cry that may send the
rest of the meerkats scurrying to
safety.
The eyesight of the meerkat is quite
good. A meerkat sentinel can look
straight up and at the blazing South
African sky to keep a hawk in sight.
Dog and cat owners know that their
pets cannot see color; indeed, most
mammals except man and the other
primates, cannot see color. But recent
studies have shown that meerkats can.
Interestingly, though their eyesight by
day is far superior to man’s, their
eyesight by night is not as good as ours.
Zoo Obtains
Brush-Tailed Phalanger
The brush-tailed phalanger (7richesurus
vulpecula) has a reputation among
zoologists as one of the world’s most
adaptable mammals. It is one of a very
few native Australian animals to have
actually profited from man’s profound
alteration of that continent’s environ-
ment. This long-eared, bushy-tailed
marsupial has taken up residence in
city parks and suburban gardens. In
this respect it is reminiscent of the only
marsupial native to the United States,
the opossum—another highly adaptable
species.
A new exhibit at the Small Mammal
House contains a male brush-tailed
phalanger. Branches have been pro-
vided, and the resident phalanger dis-
plays the species’ excellent climbing
ability. The brush-tailed phalanger has
a hairless patch on its chest where
scent-glands are located. The Zoo’s
phalanger can be seen rubbing this area
on the branches in its cage, marking the
cage as his territory.
The phalanger’s successful coexistence
with man in Australia is even more
remarkable since man has hardly been a
good neighbor. Until recently brush-
tailed phalangers were hunted in large
numbers for their furs.
The brush-tailed phalanger is one of the most common Australian mammals.
The brush-tailed phalanger is omniv-
orous, feeding in the wild on leaves,
flowers, fruits, seeds, and insects. It
may take some vertebrate prey, such as
nestling birds. It is most common in
forested areas, but is sometimes found
in open and even near-desert regions.
Zoo Receives Grison
“Grison” rhymes with ‘‘bison’’; but the
grison and the bison have little else in
common except that both are mam-_
mals. Anyone who is curious to know
what a grison is like can visit the pair
that have recently taken up residence
at the Small Mammal House.
Grison are South American relatives of
our weasels, badgers and skunks. In
10
fact the grison looks like a combination
of all three. It is low-slung likey a
badger, slender like a weasel, ana
colored rather like a skunk.
The coloration of the grison’s fur is of
particular. interest. There is a. wimite
longitudinal stripe on top of its head,
its back is gray, and its underside is
black.
Like its relatives, the grison preys on
rodents and other small mammals. For-
merly, chinchilla hunters in South
America kept pet grison, which they
trained to go down chinchilla burrows
and chase the rodents out. Grison are
also fond of fruit, and they can climb
trées. to get it. Since they are capa
swimmers, they may even do some
fishing.
11
I’ve been having lunch with a lizard. As
I’ve been eating my sandwich, /guana
iguana on the other side of the glass has
been busy attacking and devouring a
fat orange slice. Cautiously he ap-
proached the feed tray, gingerly he
lowered his head, and then, opening his
mouth as wide as possible, he at-
tempted to surround the piece of
orange. He almost got the whole thing
in with one bite; but as he raised his
head there were still corners of juicy
orange jutting out on either side. He
opened his mouth again and, throwing
his head back, managed to shift the
orange backward and down his throat
in a gulp. | couldn’t do that, as much
bigger than the iguana as | am.
Some people don’t like to watch rep-
tiles eat. Fortunately | am not one of
them; but watching this iguana | think
| can guess the reason. Reptiles bolt
their food. They use their teeth only to
take hold of food—or at most to bite
off a piece of it—but never to chew.
The general rule with reptiles is that
anything that can fit into the mouth
can be swallowed whole.
When I examine the iguana closely, |
can see why he doesn’t chew, why
indeed he can’t chew. He has no lips,
for instance, as indeed no reptile has.
Of course his mouth has edges; but a
real lip is a lot more than just a place
where the face ends and the mouth
cavity begins. Like those of most other
mammals, my lips have a sphincter
muscle enabling me to close them tight
while my jaws continue to move up
and down behind them. In other
words, because | have lips, | can chew
with my mouth closed. But a reptile’s
“‘lips’’—such as they are—of necessity
move up and down with his jaws. He
has no sphincter muscle around his
mouth. I recall a shot of novocaine
(injected near my upper incisors) that
paralyzed my lips for a few hours. A
lizard’s lips are stiff like that all the
time.
My more refined mammalian eating
habits are not, it should be noted, a
mere matter of table manners. After
all, | had ancestors that were reptiles
and couldn’t chew. The process by
which each successive generation of my
reptilian ancestors approached the state
of being mammalian went hand in hand
with a process of becoming able to
chew. And they had to be able to chew
to be mammals. For mammals have
higher body temperatures and faster
heart rates than reptiles and to main-
tain these they need quick energy.
Chewing is a means of speeding the
The indigo snake on the left is a typical reptile; all of its teeth are the same. The rattlesnake on the right
has two front teeth modified as venom-injecting fangs.
A juvenile binturong eating a banana.
conversion of food to energy. The large
python that has swallowed—whole—a
rabbit or young pig is content to wait
on a much slower process of digestion.
He may lie nearly comatose for two
weeks or more after such a meal, doing
nothing much but digesting and assimi-
lating.
Snakes don’t break up their food in
any way. Their jaws can even be
unhinged so that they can further
expand their mouths to get food down.
Often other reptiles do break up their
food—not by chewing but by other
essentially extraoral means. A lizard
may bite a chunk off a fruit that is too
big to get into his mouth whole. A
snapping turtle can tear a chunk off a
fish he has captured by grasping a part
of the fish in his mouth and pushing
strongly against the rest of it with one
13
of his forepaws. A river-full of croco-
diles, playing tug of war with a carcass,
can rapidly dismember it into swallow-
able pieces.
A further important difference be-
tween mammals and reptiles concerns
the matter of teeth. Because they
chew, the teeth of mammals are like
the teeth of no other vertebrates.
Throughout each of his jaws the iguana
has only one kind of tooth—a basic,
all-purpose iguana-tooth. Only a few
reptiles have more than one kind of
tooth. A rattlesnake’s teeth, for in-
stance, are all basically the same except
for two. greatly enlarged venom-
injecting fangs in front. Mammals, of
course, typically have four different
kinds of teeth located in different parts
of the mouth and performing different
functions: incisors, canines, premolars,
and molars. Reptiles’ teeth are essen-
tially concerned with the function of
getting food and not with that of
breaking it down. This is true of a
rattlesnake’s teeth that inject venom
into the prey or of a lizard’s teeth that
bite off a piece of fruit. In the typical
mammal, on the other. hand, some
The prairie dog has forepaws adapted for taking hold of food.
teeth retain a food-getting function
while others take on the new function
of chewing.
Though there are many peculiar things
about man that set him apart from
other mammals, the way he uses his
teeth is not one of them. As with us so
with most other mammals the incisors
and canines are food-getters, while the
molars and premolars are usually grind-
ers. A rabbit, for instance, uses his
prominent incisors to chop off grass,
then grinds it with his molars before
swallowing. | use my incisors and
molars in the same way when I eat
corn-on-the-cob. The canines and even
a premolar may also at times be used
for food-getting. When I bite into an
apple, | usually don’t bite straight on
but a little sideways. Thus | enhance
the biting power of my incisors by
enlisting an upper canine too—and
maybe even a premolar as well.
In members of the cat family—the most
efficient predators that have ever
walked the earth—the food-getting
function of the canines has become
particularly important. A cat’s canine
can bite at the base of a mouse’s skull
with such deadly accuracy that the
rodent’s spinal cord is _ instantly
severed.
Mammals are immensely varied, as all
zoogoers are aware; thus, it is not
surprising that some mammals’ mouths
are set up very differently from the
typical pattern. Anteaters, for instance,
have no teeth at all. The long, sticky
tongue does all the food-getting neces-
sary, and ants and termites can be
swallowed unchewed.
The elephant has only six functioning
teeth at a time. Two of these are upper
15
incisors that have become the tusks.
The tusks, of course, have no food-
getting utility, being instead weapons
and status symbols. Food-getting is
relegated to that unique elephantine
structure, the trunk. For grinding, the
elephant has huge molars, but only
four of them are in use at any given
time. Actually the elephant has twelve
molars like most mammals, including
man. But in the elephant all twelve are
not in place at once. Rather there is
only one molar in place at any given
time on each side of each jaw—for a
total of four. Another molar is waiting
in the wings to replace each of these
four as it gets worn down. The replace-
ment process continues until all twelve
teeth alloted to the elephant have been
worn down. An jold elephant that has
used up his twelve teeth is in real
trouble—his species not having invented
false ones.
In this connection it is worth remark-
ing (less we become excessively proud
of our superior mammalian teeth) that
reptiles have one sort of dental superi-
ority over mammals. Reptilian teeth
are replaceable ad infinitum, being
periodically shed and replaced through-
out life. There are no toothless old
crocodiles as there are toothless old
elephants and toothless old men.
While mammals modified reptilian
teeth in their own ways, another off-
shoot of reptiles—the birds—dispensed
with teeth altogether. Birds also do not
chew, and their bills are in essence
food-getters only. Many birds eat only
insects and so do not need to chew for
the same reason that the mammalian
anteater does not need to chew. Seed-
eating birds have evolved a way of
“chewing” inside the stomach itself.
Part of the stomach has evolved into a
highly muscular gizzard. Seed-eating
birds go around swallowing pebbles and
bits of grit. These ‘gizzard stones’’
remain in the gizzard, where they are
used to grind up seeds.
To “eat like a bird,” in folk parlance, Is
to eat very little. Actually for their size
birds eat quite a lot. This is especially
true of smaller birds, who have to
maintain body temperatures and heart
rates even higher than those of mam-
mals. A study of food consumption by
all the birds and mammals of a two-
thousand acre forest indicated that the
total bird population consumed food
equal to about 25 percent of its body
weight daily, while the mammals con-
sumed about 20 percent of their body
weight daily.
The phrase about eating like a bird,
however, originated before people
realized how extensive a bird’s daily
The dorcas gazelle is a cud-chewer.
consumption is. Birds seem to eat very
little if you look only at individual
meals. Most such meals consist only of
one insect or one seed. It is only rarely
that a seed- or insect-eating bird will.
come across such foodstuffs in great
numbers or all in one place. He must
16
usually search for them one at a time.
Besides, the fact that birds have bills
instead of wide reptilian mouths forces
them to take their food bit by bit. So it
is that most birds seem to be almost
always searching for food, as did a
grackle | watched all one afternoon
gradually working his way up a hillside,
turning over dead leaf after dead leaf.
He’s a far cry from the man who eats
only three meals a day—or from the
iguana at the Zoo who is prefectly
content to be fed only two days a
week, or from a python that gets by on
one meal a month.
The Private Life of the Rabbit
By R. M. Lockley 152 pages
MacMillan Publishing Co., Inc.,
New York. $6.95
R.M. Lockley is the most British of
naturalists. In his love of detail, in
his empathy with the small and
unobtrusive inhabitants of the
earth, Lockley stands with a long
tradition among nature-writers on
that crowded island. Lockley has
specialized in patient observation of
the daily lives of wild creatures that
would seldom appear glamorous to
most of us or even worthy of a second
glance. What less promising subject
for study could have been imagined
than the Manx-shearwater—a
dull-hued seabird that sleeps all day
ina hole in the ground? Yet, as
Lockley showed in his classic
Shearwaters (now regrettably out
of print) the Manx-shearwater has
an engaging family life and is capable
of staggering feats of homing and
navigation.
The rabbit is a natural subject for
such a writer, and Lockley’s
acquaintance with rabbits Is a
long-standing one. He raised
domestic rabbits as a teenager.
As a young man, he tried for a
time to make a living trapping
wild European rabbits, the species
from which the domestic rabbit
is descended. Then, in the early
1950’s a rabbit-disease known as
myxomatosis began to decimate
wild rabbit populations throughout
Europe. The Nature Conservancy
called on Lockley to investigate
its spread.
The myxomatosis virus—or “‘mixie”’
as British country folk began to call
it for short—originated in Brazil. The
native rabbits there had developed
partial immunity to the disease, as
man has to such diseases as the
measles. But the European rabbits
were without the same protection.
In many European countries rabbit
populations had reached plague
proportions in the early 1950’s. In
desperation, a frustrated French
landowner introduced myxomatosis.
Farmers applauded the spread of the
virus; but, in England, the Nature
Conservancy was concerned about
what would happen if the species
were entirely wiped out.
Rabbits are active, vigorous consumers
of just about any grass or herbaceous
plant; and in their absence the
entire character of the vegetation of
the countryside might change. As it
happened, rabbit populations in
Europe bottomed out, and the spread
of the disease was halted. Lockley
found that the virus was transmitted
by minute rabbit fleas, and thus
spread quickly only under over-
crowded conditions. Still, the Nature
Conservancy was interested in
continuing its investigation of the
over-all impact of the rabbit on the
environment. An important part
of this work was a detailed study of
the rabbit’s social life. With
characteristic patience Lockley
devoted three full years to an
investigation of rabbit social
behavior, and what he found forms
the heart of this book.
It took some ingenuity to invade
the privacy of a species as shy as the
rabbit. Rabbits are so secretive
that it would have been impossible to
observe their behavior in a completely
wild state. Thus Lockley did most of
his studies on groups enclosed in
one-to-two-acre rabbit-proof fences.
Such enclosures were large enough
to provide all the rabbits’ food needs.
In addition Lockley devised an
ingenious glass-walled burrow that
permitted him to observe the
below-ground life of one of his
rabbit families.
Not surprisingly, rabbits are very
social animals. You have to be if you
spend a lot of time huddled ina
burrow with others of your kind.
Each burrow system or “‘warren”’ is
ruled by a dominant male, in
conjunction with a dominant female.
There are subordinate males and
subordinate females, usually
offspring of the dominant pair.
esos
NARROW
CONCRETE LINED
PASSAGES
4
eA
i]
TMI
MM
Though he seems to form a special
bond with the ruling female, the
dominant male has access to any
other female in the warren.
Subordinate males may mate with
subordinate females—but only when
the boss is not looking. A dominant
male may leave his warren to conquer
a new, better warren. Or he may be
ousted by an ambitious subordinate
or stranger. But the female rabbit is
a ‘‘homebody.”’ Once she has given
birth in a warren, she stays there.
Breeding females come along with the
warren when a new male takes charge.
For Lockley, the social life of the
rabbit is a source of numerous insights
into our own behavior. He finds in
their territorial and domestic
squabbles illuminating parallels with
our own. As Lockley puts it:
“Humans are so rabbit.”’ In other
respects, the rabbit has adaptations
we might well envy. In times of
overpopulation a pregnant female
rabbit can reabsorb her fetus. The
Diagram (elevation) of artificial warren for observing rabbits underground
18
species has evolved a built-in
mechanism to cope with its own
proverbially prolific breeding
potential.
It is perhaps unfortunate that the
publisher has felt it necessary to
preface the American edition of
this excellent book with an
introduction by the author of a
bestseller of no particular merit.
But if it succeeds in its purpose of
introducing one of the very best
living nature writers to a larger
audience, Macmillan’s strategem
is all to the good. At least Mr.
Richard Adams (author of
Watership Down) has the good grace
to sound embarrassed in his
introduction and to be brief.
However, should any reader
hesitate between buying Mr.
Adams’s book and Mr. Lockley’s,
| would assuredly recommend the
latter—and not for reasons of
price alone.
The Incredible Egg: A Billion Year
Journey
by Dal Stivens. 373 pages
Weybright and Talley, New YorR
$9.95.
We all come from eggs. You or |
didn’t hatch from one personally, of
course, but we both have ancestors
that did. Smug in our higher-
mammalian ability to bring an
embryo to term in the security of
a womb, we may not realize how
much we owe to eggs.
Dal Stivens documents and dramatizes
that debt in his new book. He not
only describes what an egg is and
what goes on inside it but he provides
an informal introduction to a wide
spectrum of animals, from fish to
bird of paradise, from toad to
platypus. These animals all lay eggs,
and they are all our fellow vertebrates.
There are the labyrinth fishes, for
instance, that float their eggs in nests
of air-bubbles. There’s a treefrog in
the Amazon valley that steals beeswax
and uses it to make a watertight pool
in a hollow tree in which she lays her
eggs. And there’s the cave swiftlet of
Asia which builds its nest entirely of
dried salivary mucous—the same nest
used by the Chinese as the basis of -
bird’s nest soup.
The reviewer confesses to a passion
for random facts and holds that of all
sorts of information one can have,
useless information is the best. For
those who share these prejudices, a
book like The /ncredible Egg isa
rare delight. Did you know, for
instance, that when you are eating
caviar you are eating some of the
most primitive vertebrate eggs you
can find? That the largest bird’s
eggs ever, those of the extinct
elephant birds of Madagascar, were
over a foot long and equal in
volume to 100 hen’s eggs?
Stivens shows that not only is the egg
an object deserving our admiration
but that it holds the key to the 500
million year drama of vertebrate
evolution. In fact, to look inside an
egg is to see that epic procession of
life forms being tived again in
summary. And in the egg’s nourishing
fluid the emergence of all life from
the ocean is re-enacted.
Something to think about at breakfast.
HONZINEWS
Arundel Delivers
Annual Report
by Arthur W. Arundel
FONZ President
Despite the untamed ecomony, a wild
winter-long energy crisis, and on-going
Zoo construction, which combined to
reduce visitors and spending at the Zoo
by 20 percent, the Friends were able to
allocate more funds than ever before
to educational services and programs.
Sabin Robbins joined us in April as our
new Executive Director, replacing
Warren Iliff who joined the National
Zoo administration in charge of visitor
services. With addition to the FONZ
staff of a business manager, efficient
supervision and management of our
gift shops, substantial economies
achieved in running our safari trains,
and the cost-cutting competence of
every FONZ staffer, this has been a
year of maximizing efficiencies and
sound financial management.
The real story though, is not measured
in dollars but in our success in telling
the Zoo conservation story and doing
all in our power to assure a meaningful
and quality experience for the Zoo-
goers. Naturally, with more members
we are better able to achieve this in-
ternal goal. | am happy to report that
we now have more than 5,000 individual
and family memberships—more than
double that of last year.
Thru free television public service films,
free radio announcements, metro bus
cards, window displays, and letters of
invitation, area residents are being urged
to join “THE WILDEST CLUB IN
TOWN.” To convince members and
prospective members alike that the
Wildest Club in Town is also the Best
Bargain in Town, we have greatly ex-
panded the number of member activ-
ities, most of which are free or at very
minimal cost.
In addition to the most successful ZOO-
NIGHT ever last spring which included
live animal demonstrations, behind-the-
scenes tours, and guest TV star Bill
Burrud, other FREE events this year
will include three outstanding wildlife
film programs, even a FREE champagne
reception in the Elephant House to
premiere a major Zoo exhibit. We have
already taken members on two fall field
trips to Patuxent Wildlife Refuge and
Lion Country Safari; and there will be
more inexpensive field trips in the
spring. Also during the fall and winter
months we are offering three out-
standing illustrated lectures by such
world renowned wildlife experts as Dr.
George Schaller.
Starting in January we will repeat our
successful series of ZOO CLASSES
for all ages with other special classes
scheduled for the spring.
One of our oldest and most impor-
tant volunteer programs, windowshop
sales, continued its grand and imagina-
tive work.
Our most unusual volunteer program
is that of preg-watching. | am happy
to report that a FONZ volunteer was
there at the birth of a rare sable ante-
lope this summer. Our longest preg-
watch occurred last Christmas when a
faithful corp of volunteers overcame
snowy weather outside and mice in-
side to watch Femelle the gorilla thru
the holiday nights. Although Femelle
got bigger and bigger every week, Zoo
authorities reported after several
months that we had been watching not The Friends also assisted the Zoo in its
a pregnant gorilla, but just a fat one. multi-faceted role as social host toa
diverse selection of visiting groups and
In spring and summer months, our dignitaries. We have hosted the Wonshu
volunteers aided the Zoo in important Chinese Ballet Troupe, convention dele-
behavior studies on the giant pandas, gates from the American Institute of
Patrick, the baby Indian rhino, and the Architects and American Booksellers
cheetahs. Association, children of Congressmen
Al Perry, the Zoo’s elephant trainer, with Shanti and Ambika, performing for the Friends’ Annual Meeting.
21
and foreign diplomats, and groups of
senior citizens, handicapped children,
and inner-city youths. In nearly every
case, we provided free train rides, free
souvenirs, and refreshments, and we
hope a wonderful memory of their
Zoo Visit.
The Friends also published and dis-
tributed free some 200,000 Zoo bro-
chures and maps to arriving zoogoers.
And mailed out hundreds of packets
to those requesting information on the
Zoo and its animals. When you total
up all this and the volunteer hours
given by guides, docents, information
booth aides, and shop sales assistants,
the Friends contributed over a quarter
of a million dollars to the Zoo’s educa-
tional programs just last year.
Finally, this is to report that our most
exciting educational project, production
of a 20 minute color orientation film
about the National Zoo, has been com-
pleted. Its educational impact will be
immense in the months and years to
come as school children and groups
around Washington, indeed around the
nation and world, are given a very
special look at the behind-the-scenes
workings of a Zoo. This film is already
being hailed as perhaps the finest
Zoo film ever done.
It has been a good year. We are confi-
dent that the Friends will meet its
rapidly expanding challenges with an
increasing level of imagination and
dedicated professionalism.
With your help thru membership sup-
port and contributions, the Friends
can continue to play a greater and
greater role in supporting wildlife
conservation thru Zoo educational
programs.
22
FONZ Ends 1974 on the
Financial Upswing
by Dr. Stephen Hosmer
FONZ Treasurer
| shall be reporting on the 12-month
period ending September 30, 1974,
and all comparisons are with the 12-
month period ending September 30,
hoy.
Let me now briefly touch on the high-
lights of a generally very good finan-
cial year for the FONZ.
Despite an estimated 20% decrease in
attendance at the Zoo during the win-
ter and early spring due to the fuel
shortage, our overall performance
during the past 12 months was still
superior to a comparable period of a
year ago. During the last 12 months
net profits from all FONZ operations
amounted to about $67,000, a 15%
increase over 1972-1973.
This rise in net revenues, however,
does not reflect the whole story in
that the net increase occurred ata
time when FONZ expenditures for
membership, Zoo service and educa-
tional activities were also growing
substantially. For example:
e Total expenditures for membership
increased by 25% during the period
from about $34,000 to almost
$43,000. This increase was used to
support the initiation of a major
campaign to attract new members
to the FONZ.
e Weare particularly pleased to an-
nounce that we were able to increase
expenditures for education by almost
90% from a total of about $28,000
in the previous period to some
$53,000 over the last 12 months. The
bulk of this added expenditure has
been used to finance new teaching
materials for local school systems,
increased activities for the Junior Zoo
_ Aide program, and, of course, the Zoo
film which you will see tonight. Total
cost for our film to date has been
about $33,000, of which over $20,500
has been financed by the FONZ and
$12,500 by a grant from the Morris
and Gwendolyn Cafritz Foundation.
We are most appreciative for the
Cafritz Foundation’s generous support.
Another development worthy of note
has been the marked improvement in
our cash and inventory positions during
the past 12 months. As compared to
September 30, 1973, our cash-on-hand
and savings have increased by over
$34,000. As of the end of September,
our cash position and savings amounted
to some $93,000. During the same peri-
od our gift shop inventory has increased
by $16,000 to a present inventory level
of about $95,000. Finally, our outstand-
ing debt (including that owed on the
purchase of the trackless trains) has been
cut in half from around $46,000 to the
present $23,000 owed.
To summarize, 1974 finds the FONZ in
an improved financial position and
increasingly capable of fulfilling our
educational responsibilities. Credit for
this improvement is of course due to
our dedicated and resourceful FONZ
staff, which has successfully worked to
hold down operating expenses while
increasing revenues. In addition to the
fine work of Sabin Robbins, particular
credit must go to our FONZ business
manager, Dennis Baker, and to Mrs.
Farnsworth, our gift shop manager and
greatest producer of revenue. Many
others have also contributed.
FONZ CALENDAR 1975
February 8, 10:30 a.m.
Wildlife films—‘‘ Baobab: Portrait
Ov a nee”
and ‘‘Mzima: Portrait of a Spring”’ in
Baird Auditorium, Museum of Natural
History (10th & Constitution Ave NW)
PRE
February 13, 8:00 p.m.
Greg McMillan, film-lecture, “The Living
Jungle” at Baird Auditorium; $2.00; $3.00
March 18, 8:00 p.m.
Karl Maslowski, film-lecture, “‘Wildlife
by Day and Night” in New Lecture Hall
at American University; $2.00; $3.00
April 5, 10:30 a.m.
Wildlife films from Australia in Baird
Auditorium, FREE
PARKING
Baird Auditorium—free parking available
in the West Parking Lot alongside the
Natural History Building. The entrance
is on Constitution Ave between 1 2th
St and the building.
New Lecture Hal/l—free parking available
off Nebraska Avenue directly across from
the campus.